Смекни!
smekni.com

False Memory Essay Research Paper Saetern ChrisPsychologyPPMark (стр. 1 из 2)

False Memory Essay, Research Paper

Saetern Chris

Psychology

PP:Mark Art

Spring,99

TTH 5PM

Saetern@usa.net

FALSE MEMORY SYNDROME – IS IT POSSIBLE OR NOT?

Memory is the mental faculty of retaining and recalling past experiences. A

repressed memory is one that is retained in the sub conscious mind, where

one is not aware of it but where it can still affect both conscious thoughts

and behavior.

When memory is distorted or confabulated, the result can be what has been

called the False Memory Syndrome: a condition in which a person’s identity

and interpersonal relationships are entered around a memory of traumatic

experience which is objectively false but in which the person strongly

believes. Note that the syndrome is not characterized by false memories as

such. We all have memories that are inaccurate. Rather, the syndrome may be

diagnosed when the memory is so deeply ingrained that it orients the

individual’s entire personality and lifestyle, in turn disrupting all sorts

of other adaptive behaviors. The analogy to personality disorder is

intentional. False memory syndrome is especially destructive because the

person assiduously avoids confrontation with any evidence that might

challenge the memory. Thus it takes on a life of its own, encapsulated and

resistant to correction. The person may become so focused on the memory that

he or she may be effectively distracted from coping with real problems in

his or her life.

– John F. Kihlstrom, Ph.D.

There are many models which try to explain how memory works. Nevertheless,

we do not know exactly how memory works. One of the most questionable models

of memory is the one which assumes that every experience a person has had is

‘recorded’ in memory and that some of these memories are of traumatic events

too terrible to want to remember. These terrible memories are locked away in

the sub conscious mind, i.e. repressed, only to be remembered in adulthood

when some triggering event opens the door to the unconscious. And, both

before and after the repressed memory is remembered, it causes physical and

mental disorders in a person.

Some people have made an effort to explain their pain, even cancer, as

coming from repressed memories of incest in the body. Scientists have

studied related phenomenon such as people whose hands bleed in certain

religious settings. Presumably such people, called stigmatics, “are not

revealing unconscious memories of being crucified as young children, but

rather are demonstrating a fascinating psychogenic anomaly that springs from

their conscious fixation on the suffering of Christ. Similarly, it is

possible that conscious fixation on the idea that one was sexually abused

might increase the frequency of some physical symptoms, regardless of

whether or not the abuse really occurred.”(Lindsay & Read, 1994)

This view of memory has two elements: (1) the accuracy element and (2) the

causal element. The reason this model is questionable is not because people

don’t have unpleasant or painful experiences they would rather forget, nor

is it claiming that children often experience both wonderful and brutal

things for which they have no conceptual or linguistic framework and hence

are incapable of understanding them, much less relating it to others. It is

questionable because this model maintains that because (a) one is having

problems of functioning as a healthy human being and (b) one remembers being

abused as a child that therefore (A) one was abused as a child and (B) the

childhood abuse is the cause of one’s adulthood problems.

There is no evidence that supports the claim that we remember everything

that we experience. In fact, there is plenty of evidence to support the

claim that it is impossible for us to even attend to all the perceptual

elements of any given experience, much less to recall them all. There is no

evidence to support the claim that all memories of experiences happened as

they remembered to have happened or that they have even happened at all. And

there is no evidence to support the claim that subjective certainty about

the accuracy of memories or the vividness of memories significantly

correlates with accuracy. Finally, the claim of a causal connection between

abuse and health or behavior does not warrant concluding that ill health,

mental or physical, is a ’sign’ of having been abused.

This model is the basis for a number of pseudoscientific works on child

abuse by self-proclaimed experts such as Ellen Bass, E. Sue Blum, Laura

Davis, Beverly Engel, Beverly Holman, Wendy Maltz and Mary Jane Williams.

Through communal reinforcement many empirically unsupported notions,

including the claim that about half of all women have been sexually abused,

get treated as a ‘fact’ by many people. Psychologist Carol Tavris writes

In what can only be called an incestuous arrangement, the authors of these

books all rely on one another’s work as supporting evidence for their own;

they all recommend one another’s books to their readers. If one of them

comes up with a concocted statistic–such as “more than half of all women

survivors of childhood sexual trauma “– the numbers are traded like

baseball cards, reprinted in every book and eventually enshrined as fact.

Thus the cycle of misinformation, faulty statistics and unvalidated

assertions maintains itself. (Tavris, 1993)

The only difference between this group of experts and say, a group of

physicists is that the child abuse experts have achieved their status as

authorities not by scientific training but by either (a) experience [they

were victims themselves or they have treated victims of abuse in their

capacity as social workers] or (b) they wrote a book on child abuse. The

child abuse experts are not trained in scientific research which is not a

comment on their ability to write or to do therapy, but which does seem to

be one reason for their scientific illiteracy. (Tavris, 1993)

Here are a few of the unproved, unscientifically researched notions that

are being bandied around by these child abuse experts: One, if you doubt

that you were abused as a child or think that it might be your imagination,

this is a sign of ‘post-incest syndrome’. Two, if you can not remember any

specific instances of being abused, but still have a feeling that something

abusive happened to you, ‘it probably did’. Three, when a person can not

remember his or her childhood or have very fuzzy memories, ‘incest must

always be considered as a possibility’. And four, ‘If you have any suspicion

at all, if you have any memory, no matter how vague, it probably really

happened. It is far more likely that you are blocking the memories, denying

it happened’.

There have been many symptoms suggested as indicators of past abuse. These

symptoms range from headaches to irritable bowls. In fact, one psychologist

compiled a list of over 900 different symptoms that had been presented as

proof of a history of abuse. When he reviewed the professional literature,

he found that not one of the symptoms could be shown to be an inclusive

indication of a history of abuse. Given the lack of consistent scientific

evidence, therapists must be careful in declaring that abuse has infact

occurred. (London, 1995)

Whole industries have been built up out of the hysteria that inevitably

accompanies charges of the sexual abuse of children. Therapists who are

supposed to help children recover from the trauma of the abuse are hired to

interrogate the child, in order to find out if they have been abused. But

all too often the therapist suggests the abuse to the child and the child

has ‘memories’ of being abused, but no rational person should find a parent

or caretaker guilty on the basis of such tainted testimony. [note 1]

Increasingly throughout the continent, grown children under going

therapeutic programs have come to believe that they suffer from “repressed

memories” of incest and sexual abuse. While some reports of incest and

sexual abuse are surely true, these decade delayed memories are too often

the result of False Memory Syndrome caused by a disastrous “therapeutic”

program. False Memory Syndrome has a devastating effect on the victim and

typically produces a continuing dependency on the very program that creates

the syndrome. False Memory Syndrome proceeds to destroy the psychological

well being not only of the primary victim but through false accusations of

incest and sexual abuse other members of the primary victim’s family. The

American Medical Association considers recovered memories of childhood

sexual abuse to be of uncertain authenticity, which should be subject to

external verification. The use of recovered memories is fraught with

problems of potential misapplication.[note 2]

The dangers of this model are apparent: not only are false memories treated

as real memories, but real memories of real abuse may be treated as false

memories and may provide real abusers with a believable defense. In the end,

no one benefits from encouraging a belief in memory which is unfounded.

Whatever the theory of memory one advocates, if it does not entail examining

corroborating evidence and attempting to independently verify claims of

recollected abuse, it is a theory which will cause more harm than good.

Carl Jung, an early Freudian disciple and later heretic, extended this

model of memory by adding another area of repressed memories to the

unconscious mind, an area that was not based on individual past experiences

at all: the “collective” unconscious. The collective unconscious is the

repository for acts and mental patterns shared either by members of a

culture or universally by all humans. Under certain conditions these

manifest themselves as archetype: images, patterns and symbols, that are

often seen in dreams or fantasies and that appear as themes in mythology,

religion and fairy tales. The Archetype of the Archetype Model can be traced

back to Plato’s various beliefs about the eidos. (Forms of reality which

were variously described by Plato but always were held up as ‘more real’

than the world of sense experience which, in some way, was always held up as

inferior to and dependant on the eidos.)

The Platonic Model avoids the problem of determining whether or not a

memory is accurate by claiming that the memory is not of a personal

experience at all. It also confuses several types of mental states. It

completely blurs the distinction between dream states and conscious states

by eliminating the difference between remembering a sense experience one

actually had and remembering a sense experience one never actually had. This

model gives validity to every fantasy and desire. If one is clever, though,

one can destroy the first model with the second one. For example, a Jungian

could claim that the repressed memories of all those who are now blaming

their current troubles on forgotten and repressed memories of child abuse,

are not memories of actual abuse but of an Archetype, the Abused Child

Archetype. The story of Hansel and Gretel might be pulled in for

“scientific” support of the idea. Unsupported assertions might be made

regarding the unconscious desire of all children to be loved by their

parents: as children, love could only be understood in terms of ego

gratification, but as adults love is understood primarily in sexual terms.

Because of the incest taboo, we can not bear the thought of wanting to be

loved sexually by our parents, so this desire must be expressed in a

perverse and inverse way: our parents love us sexually. But there is no

evidence for this based upon our past or current relationship with our

parents, so the mind creates the evidence by remembering being sexually

abused as a child. Thus, the memory we have as adults of being sexually

abused by our parents is actually the expression of the universal desire to

be loved by our mother and father. It has nothing to do with any real

experience; it has everything to do with a universal human desire. It also

serves as a convenient excuse to absolve us of all responsibility for our

failures and incompetence. The reason we are so screwed up is because our

parents screwed us!

How accurate and reliable is memory? We’re often wrong in thinking we

accurately remember things. Studies on memory have shown that we often

construct our memories after the fact, that we are susceptible to

suggestions from others that help us fill in the gaps in our memories of

certain events. (Hyman, Jr., Husband & Billings, 1995) That is why, for

example, a police officer investigating a crime should not show a picture of

a single individual to a victim and ask if the victim recognizes the

assailant. If the victim is then presented a line up and picks out the

individual whose picture the victim had been shown, there is no way of

knowing whether the victim is remembering the assailant or the picture.

Another interesting fact about memory is that studies have shown that there

is no significant correlation between the subjective feeling of certainty a

person has about memory and that memory being accurate. Also, contrary to

what many believe, hypnosis does not aid memory’s accuracy because subjects

are extremely suggestible while under hypnosis. (Loftus, 1980) It is

possible to create false memories in people’s minds by suggestion.

The mind does not record every detail of an event, but only a few features;

we fill in the rest on what “must have been.” For an event to make it to

long term storage, a person has to perceive it, encode it and rehearse it

–tell about it– or it decays. (This seems to be the major mechanism behind

childhood amnesia, the fact that children do not develop long term memory

until roughly age three.) Otherwise, research finds, even emotional

experiences we are sure we will never forget –the Kennedy assassination,

the Challenger explosion– will fade from memory, and errors will creep into

the account that remain.(Travis, 1993)

Research articles and court testimony confirm the wide spread use of memory

enhancement techniques, in the belief that these will help recover accurate

“memories”. These techniques include hypnosis, sodium amytal, dream

interpretation, guided imagery, journaling, body massages, participation in

survivor groups and reading of self help books. In the summer of 1993, the

American Medical Association passed a resolution warning of the dangers of

misapplication in the use of these techniques. In June of 1994 they issued a

warning about all recovered memories. Both the AMA and the American

Psychiatric Association have stated:

..there is no completely accurate way of determining the validity of reports

in the absence of corroborating information.[note 3]

The problem with the practices mentioned above is that when they are used

they increase the risk of influence and suggestibility.

Why would someone remember something so horrible if it really did not

happen? This is a haunting question, but there are several possible

explanations which might shed light on some of the false memories. A

pseudomemory, for example, may be a kind of symbolic expression of troubled

family relationships. There may be a cultural climate in our society in

which the belief in the relationship between sexual abuse and individual

pathology is nurtured. It may be that in such a climate people more readily

believe things happened when they didn’t. When people enter therapy, they do

so to get better. They want to change. People also tend to look for some

explanation for why they have a problem. Clients come to trust the person

they have chosen to help them. Because they are trying to get better,

clients tend to rely on the therapist’s opinion. If the therapist believes

that the reason that the client has a problem is because of some past

trauma, and especially if the therapist believes that the patient will not

get better unless he or she remembers the trauma, the patient will work to

find what he or she thinks is a trauma memory in order to get better.

Richard Ofshe, Ph.D. and Ethan Watters noted that, “No one — not the

patients, therapists, parents or critics of recovered memory therapy –

question that this therapy is an intensely difficult and painful experience.

That the pain of therapy is real should not be accepted, however, as an

argument that the memories uncovered are accurate. One’s emotional reaction